Javascript Menu by Deluxe-Menu.com
MindPapers is now part of PhilPapers: online research in philosophy, a new service with many more features.
 
 Compiled by David Chalmers (Editor) & David Bourget (Assistant Editor), Australian National University. Submit an entry.
 
   
click here for help on how to search

8.6a. Consciousness and Language (Consciousness and Language on PhilPapers)

See also:
Arbib, Michael A. (2001). Co-evolution of human consciousness and language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 929:195-220.   (Cited by 18 | Google | More links)
Arbib, Michael A. (1972). Consciousness: The secondary role of language. Journal of Philosophy 64 (5):579-591.   (Cited by 2 | Google | More links)
Bailey, W. (1986). Consciousness and action/motion theories of communication. Western Journal of Speech Communication 50:74-86.   (Google)
Bever, Thomas G. & Townsend, David J. (2001). Some sentences on our consciousness of sentences. In Emmanuel Dupoux (ed.), Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler. MIT Press.   (Cited by 1 | Google)
Blachowicz, James A. (1997). The dialogue of the soul with itself. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4 (4-5):485-508.   (Cited by 10 | Google)
Bridgeman, Bruce (1992). On the evolution of consciousness and language. Psycoloquy 3 (15).   (Cited by 26 | Google | More links)
Abstract: Psychology can be based on plans, internally held images of achievement that organize the stimulus-response links of traditional psychology. The hierarchical structure of plans must be produced, held, assigned priorities, and monitored. Consciousness is the operation of the plan-executing mechanism, enabling behavior to be driven by plans rather than immediate environmental contingencies. The mechanism unpacks a single internally held idea into a series of actions. New in this paper is the proposal that language uses this mechanism for communication, unpacking an idea into a series of articulatory acts. Language comprehension uses the plan-monitoring mechanism to pack a series of linguistic events into an idea. Recursive processing results from monitoring one's own speech. Neurophysiologically, the planning mechanism is identified with higher-order motor control
Brook, Andrew (1996). Jackendoff and consciousness. Pragmatics and Cognition 4 (1):81-92.   (Cited by 1 | Google)
Carruthers, Peter (1996). The involvement of language in conscious thinking. In Language, Thought, and Consciousness. Cambridge University Press.   (Google)
Chafe, Wallace L. (1994). Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. University of Chicago Press.   (Cited by 620 | Google | More links)
Abstract: This work offers a comprehensive picture of the dynamic natures of language and consciousness that will interest linguists, psychologists, literary scholars,...
Chapman, S. B. & Ulatowska, H. K. (1997). Discourse in dementia: Considerations of consciousness. In Maxim I. Stamenov (ed.), Language Structure, Discourse, and the Access to Consciousness. John Benjamins.   (Google)
Chafe, Wallace L. (1996). How consciousness shapes language. Pragmatics and Cognition 4 (1):35-54.   (Google)
Chafe, Wallace L. (2007). Language and consciousness. In Philip David Zelazo, Morris Moscovitch & Evan Thompson (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. Cambridge.   (Cited by 121 | Google | More links)
Chafe, Wallace L. (1980). The deployment of consciousness in the construction of narrative. In Wallace L. Chafe (ed.), The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production. Ablex.   (Google)
Clowes, Robert (2007). A self-regulation model of inner speech and its role in the organisation of human conscious experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 14 (7):59-71.   (Google | More links)
Abstract: This paper argues for the importance of inner speech in a proper understanding of the structure of human conscious experience. It reviews one recent attempt to build a model of inner speech based on a grammaticization model (Steels, 2003) and compares it with a self-regulation model here proposed. This latter model is located within the broader literature on the role of language in cognition and the inner voice in consciousness. I argue that this role is not limited to checking the grammatical correctness of prospective utterances before they are spoken. Rather, it is a more broadly activity-structuring role, regulating and shaping the ongoing shape of human activity in the world. Through linking inner speech to the control of attention, I argue that the study of the functional role of inner speech should be a central area of analysis in our attempt to understand the development and qualitative character of human consciousness and that modelling can play a central role in that understanding
de Beaugrande, R. (1997). The "conscious and unconscious mind" in the theoretical discourse of modern linguistics. In Maxim I. Stamenov (ed.), Language Structure, Discourse, and the Access to Consciousness. John Benjamins.   (Google)
Ellis, Ralph D. (1996). Ray Jackendoff's phenomenology of language as a refutation of the 'appendage' theory of consciousness. Pragmatics and Cognition 4 (1):125-137.   (Google)
Fireman, Gary D.; McVay, T. E. & Flanagan, Owen J. (eds.) (2003). Narrative and Consciousness: Literature, Psychology and the Brain. Oxford University Press.   (Cited by 6 | Google | More links)
Abstract: We define our conscious experience by constructing narratives about ourselves and the people with whom we interact. Narrative pervades our lives--conscious experience is not merely linked to the number and variety of personal stories we construct with each other within a cultural frame, but is subsumed by them. The claim, however, that narrative constructions are essential to conscious experience is not useful or informative unless we can also begin to provide a distinct, organized, and empirically consistent explanation for narrative in relation to consciousness. Understanding the role of narrative in determining individual and collective consciousness has been elusive from within traditional academic frameworks. This volume argues that addressing so broad and complex a problem requires an examination from outside our insular disciplinary framework. Such an open examination would be informed by the inquiries and approaches of multiple disciplines. Recognition of the different approaches toexamining personal stories will allow for the coordination of how narrative seems (its phenomenology), with what mental labor it does (its psychology), and how it is realized (its neurobiology). Only by overcoming the boundaries erected by multiple theoretical and discursive traditions can we begin to comprehend the nature and function of narrative in consciousness. Narrative and Consciousness brings together essays by exceptional scholars and scientists in the disciplines of literary theory, psychology, and neuroscience to examine how stories are constructed, how stories structure lived experience, and how stories are rooted in material reality (the human body). The specific topics addressed include narrative in the development of conscious awareness; autobiographical narrative, fiction and the construction of self; trauma and narrative disruptions; narrative, memory and identity; and the physiological and neural substrate of narrative. It is the editors' hope that the multidisciplinary nature of this collection will challenge the reader to move beyond disciplinary confines and toward a coherent interdisciplinary dialogue
Fludernik, M. & Sell, R. D. (1995). The fictions of language and the languages of fiction: The linguistic representation of speech and consciousness. Journal of Pragmatics 24.   (Cited by 96 | Google | More links)
Glicksohn, Joseph (2001). Metaphor and consciousness: The path less taken. Journal of Mind and Behavior 22 (4):343-364.   (Google)
Gross, Steven (2009). Review of Ray Jackendoff, Language, Consciousness, Culture. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews 20095 (5).   (Google | More links)
Hofstadter, Albert (1969). On the consciousness and language of art. Philosophy East and West 19 (1):3-15.   (Google | More links)
Johnston, P. K. (1997). Battle within: Shakespeare's brain and the nature of human consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4 (4):365-73.   (Google)
Langacker, Ronald W. (1997). Consciousness, construal, and subjectivity. In Maxim I. Stamenov (ed.), Language Structure, Discourse, and the Access to Consciousness. John Benjamins.   (Cited by 8 | Google)
Lecours, A. R. (1998). Language contrivance on consciousness (and vice versa). In H. Jasper, L. Descarries, V. Castellucci & S. Rossignol (eds.), Consciousness: At the Frontiers of Neuroscience. Lippincott-Raven.   (Cited by 1 | Google | More links)
Leontiev, A. N. (2005). Lecture 13. language and consciousness. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 43 (5):5-13.   (Google)
Lichtenberg, Joseph (2002). Values, consciousness, and language. Psychoanalytic Inquiry 22 (5):841-856.   (Google | More links)
Macphail, E. (2000). The search for a mental rubicon. In C. Heyes & Ludwig Huber (eds.), The Evolution of Cognition. MIT Press.   (Cited by 7 | Google)
Marsh, James L. (1978). Consciousness and expression. Southwestern Journal of Philosophy 9:105-109.   (Google)
Markey, J. F. (1925). The place of language habits in a behavioristic explanation of consciousness. Psychological Review 32:384-401.   (Cited by 2 | Google)
Nelson, Katherine (2003). Narrative and the emergence of a consciousness of self. In Gary D. Fireman, T. E. McVay & Owen J. Flanagan (eds.), Narrative and Consciousness. Oxford University Press.   (Cited by 8 | Google | More links)
Pronko, N. H. (1987). Language with or without consciousness. In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (eds.), Cognition, Language and Consciousness: Integrative Levels. Lawrence Erlbaum.   (Google)
Ricciardelli, L. A. (1993). Two components of metalinguistic awareness: Control of linguistic processing and analysis of linguistic knowledge. Applied Psycholinguistics 14:349-367.   (Cited by 11 | Google)
Schooler, Jonathan W. & Fiore, S. M. (1997). Consciousness and the limits of language: You can't always say what you think or think what you say. In Jonathan D. Cohen & Jonathan W. Schooler (eds.), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness. Lawrence Erlbaum.   (Cited by 7 | Google)
Searle, John R. (2002). Consciousness and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.   (Cited by 43 | Google | More links)
Abstract: One of the most important and influential philosophers of the last 30 years, John Searle has been concerned throughout his career with a single overarching question: how can we have a unified and theoretically satisfactory account of ourselves and of our relations to other people and to the natural world? In other words, how can we reconcile our common-sense conception of ourselves as conscious, free, mindful, rational agents in a world that we believe comprises brute, unconscious, mindless, meaningless, mute physical particles in fields of force? The essays in this collection are all related to the broad overarching issue that unites the diverse strands of Searle's work. Gathering in an accessible manner essays available only in relatively obscure books and journals, this collection will be of particular value to professionals and upper-level students in philosophy as well as to Searle's more extended audience in such fields as psychology and linguistics
Sekhar, A. C. (1948). Language and consciousness. Indian Journal of Psychology 23:79-84.   (Google)
Sinha, Vimalendu N. (1987). Symbolic language not a pre-requisite for self-awareness. Psycho-Lingua 17:115-121.   (Google)
Smith, Thomas R. (2004). Narrative and consciousness: Review article. Journal of Consciousness Studies 11 (5-6):146-155.   (Google)
Stamenov, Maxim I. (1997). Grammar, meaning, and consciousness: What sentence structure can tell us about the structure of consciousness. In Maxim I. Stamenov (ed.), Language Structure, Discourse, and the Access to Consciousness. John Benjamins.   (Cited by 5 | Google)
Stamenov, Maxim I. (2003). Language and self-consciousness: Modes of self-presentation in language structure. In Tilo Kircher & Anthony S. David (eds.), The Self in Neuroscience and Psychiatry. Cambridge University Press.   (Cited by 10 | Google)
Stamenov, Maxim I. (2001). Language structure and the structure of consciousness: Can one find a 'common denominator' between them? In Paavo Pylkkanen & Tere Vaden (eds.), Dimensions of Conscious Experience. John Benjamins.   (Google)
Stamenov, Maxim I. (ed.) (1997). Language Structure, Discourse, and the Access to Consciousness. John Benjamins.   (Cited by 2 | Google)
Subitzky, E. (2003). I am a conscious essay. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10 (12):64-66.   (Google)